1. Introduction
Over the last two decades, the field of empirical aesthetics has offered a wealth of behavioral and neurophysiological insights into our engagement with art. A growing number of studies have considered our reactions to art’s visual properties, our emotional and cognitive responses, locations of activations in the brain related to aesthetic experience, as well as various factors which influence our reactions, such as context or individual differences (see Chatterjee and Vartanian, 2016; Pelowski and Specker, 2020 for reviews). This has led to insights into how we visually and cognitively attend to art, the neural bases for its contemplation, or when and why we might find art rewarding, arousing, or pleasurable.
At the same time, and despite this large body of research, there are still areas that have received relatively little empirical focus at both the levels of stimuli and the means of engagement: Up to now, the field has mainly assessed individual’s responses to two-dimensional visual art, involving our attention to paintings or drawings, presented on screens or as prints in the laboratory (e.g., Vartanian and Skov, 2014) or, increasingly, as genuine artworks encountered in the museum (e.g., Brieber et al., 2015). However, while again important, these studies omit an important art form—installation art (Greb et al., 2017). Despite being one of the most prevalent art forms in the contemporary art world, it involves site-specific implementations or other aesthetic decisions that shape the environment itself. It has been centrally featured in many leading art shows (e.g., see the list of winners of Golden Lions in recent Venice Biennales) and may provide particularly psychologically interesting, conceptually challenging, and emotionally moving experiences (Pelowski et al., 2018). Even more, installation art specifically anticipates an aspect of engagement that, while perhaps essential both for installations and, more generally, most art experience, has itself been all but overlooked: the role and use of the body (Kühnapfel et al., 2023; Fingerhut and Spee, forthcoming).
The importance of the body for the art experience becomes apparent when considering how we engage art in free-moving, ambulatory fashions in galleries, museums, or public spaces. Theoretical literature, in turn, is abundant in assuming a constitutive role of bodily experience. It has been argued, e.g., that somatosensation and interoception are central to aesthetic experiences (Nummenmaa and Hari, 2023) or that bodily engagement is vital for meaning-making (Brinck, 2018), as well as understanding (Kai-Kee et al., 2020; Dekeyzer, 2022). Philosophers of art and art historians have gone so far as to propose that viewers’ physical engagement with an artwork makes it more special, interesting, or worthwhile (Fingerhut, 2018) and is one of the aspects that makes art itself valuable as a human activity (Crowther, 1993; Budd, 1995). Similar arguments are made for our awareness of our bodies in the act of engagement (Montero, 2006; Shusterman, 2012; Jung et al., 2017; Brinck, 2018; Schino et al., 2021). These arguments also touch discourse in Embodied, Embedded, Extended, and Enactive approaches to cognition (4E Cognition, e.g., Varela et al., 1991; Newen et al., 2018), as well as phenomenological (e.g., Merleau-Ponty, 1962; Dufrenne, 1973) and pragmatism-based theory (e.g., Dewey, 1934, 1980), which suggests focusing on the situated and embodied nature when studying experience, as well as in somaesthetics, which recognized the body as the experiential core of perception and action and foregrounds the role of (bodily) experience in aesthetic appreciation (Shusterman, 1999). In addition, our study informs a new philosophical research project which investigates whether works of art (not limited to installation art) can be either primarily or predominantly proprioceptive in nature in that a bodily self-awareness (above, e.g., visual aspects) is required for the reception of artworks. Such artworks, among which the installation in orbit by Tomás Saraceno (see Methods for details) used in this study has been a frequently discussed example, could qualify as so-called “proprioceptive art” or “PropArt” (Schrenk, 2014; Kessels and Schrenk, 2022).
The role of the body becomes especially salient in installation art. Unlike two-dimensional visual art, installation art, due to its immersive and participatory character, is notably argued to evoke bodily awareness and require consideration of bodily experience (Best, 2002; Oliveira et al., 2003; Bishop, 2005). Specifically, it has been argued that active physical participation and heightened bodily presence or awareness might expand our appreciation modes and increase the emotional intensity of the installation art experience (Best, 2002; Bishop, 2005). Body awareness may also be key for eliciting potentially “profound”, self-reflective, and transformative reactions to installation art (Mills, 2009; Pelowski et al., 2018), which are also emerging as key topics in general discussions of art experience (Pelowski and Akiba, 2011; Pelowski, 2015; Fingerhut and Spee, forthcoming).
This suggests that installation art would be a particularly intriguing aspect and art form to investigate for empirical aesthetic research, considering elements of the body that might be key in appreciation. Many of those aspects, found more prominently in installation art settings, could also provide key insights into our general understanding of how we engage art or what factors might be considered in future aesthetic investigations. However, despite a few studies that have begun to consider some objectively measured body-related aspects, such as movement (e.g., Linden and Wagemans, 2021; Kühnapfel et al., 2023), types of interactions (Savaş et al., 2021; Szubielska et al., 2021), body posture (Kapoula et al., 2014), and embodiment as a component of emotions (Eskine et al., 2012; Schino et al., 2021; also see Cox and van Klaveren, 2022), to date, the body’s role in art experience is only a now emerging topic in empirical aesthetics. Thus, the impact such bodily experience or engagement might have on the overall art experience remains poorly understood. Even more, the subjective side of bodily experience has rarely been considered, leaving the domain of self-reports largely unexplored in general and especially with installation art.
We, therefore, lack even a basic understanding of what kind of bodily experiences people might report, how they combine, and how they relate to emotional/cognitive appreciation. Furthermore, to what extent does the individuals’ attention or awareness of their body modulates these responses? There is a need to assess these factors—for both installation art and potentially for a general model of our art experience and appreciation.
To address this gap, this study has two main aims. First, based on a theoretical review of the potential role of the body in art in general and specifically with installation art, we outline how the body is argued to be addressed by installation art. We also discuss what role the body might play in its appreciation or, more profound and transformative art experience outcomes. This resulted in a list of target items involving a range of subjective body awareness/experience factors that might play a role in individual reports of their art experiences.
Second, we empirically studied these bodily dimensions with a museum-based installation art piece. In this study, we also collected art emotional/cognitive/transformational experience factors and information on the visitor’s interpersonal differences in body awareness. We chose the site-specific installation artwork in orbit by Tomás Saraceno at the K21 (Kunstsammlung NRW) in Düsseldorf, Germany. We assessed visitors’ reported experiences related to their bodies when engaging with the installation to find support for the theoretical and art historical arguments suggesting that a focus on the body and bodily experiences could be central elements of art reception and appreciation. We further assessed how the body experience items grouped, employing an emerging technique for network analysis, providing information regarding the centrality, interconnections, and specific importance of items in defining the global bodily experience. The resulting scores assigned to participants were then used to consider whether/which components predict art experience regarding emotion, evaluation, and self-reflection/transformation.
In addition, to capture aspects involving the body that we did not pick up with our original assessment, we supplemented our quantitative list of factors with a qualitative approach, allowing participants to describe their body’s potential role freely. Finally, we explored whether individual differences in body awareness/interoception (as assessed via MAIA-2; Mehling et al., 2018) impacted the bodily experience groups defined above or moderated any relationship between bodily experience and art appreciation.
2. Review: characteristics of installation art and arguments for body-related aspects
2.1. What is installation art?
Installation art emerged in the 1960s/70s and is—broadly conceived—a spatial-temporal and site-specific art form (i.e., works are custom-made for indoor or outdoor spaces) using a wide range of materials. This often involves a rather large presentation that visitors can enter, move around in, or at least circumambulate. As such, installation art is often not only one individual “object” but an ensemble or environment. It is, therefore, often described as “theatrical,” “environmental,” “immersive,” or “experiential” (Bishop, 2005, p. 6; also see Noë, 2000, p. 128, 131), creating conditions for bodily and interactive experiences. From its mid-century inception, installation art has become a major focus of artists and contemporary museums and received increased visitor interest (Pelowski et al., 2018, 2020; Spence, 2022). Many contemporary installation art exhibitions attract thousands of visitors to wait in long lines for often short times inside the installations (Collier, 2016; Noveck, 2017), as well record visitor numbers (e.g., 160,000 visitors saw one of the world’s top-selling artists, Yayoi Kusama, “Infinity Mirror” installation; Peck, 2017, also see Neuendorf, 2018).
2.2. General arguments for how installation art anticipates the body
Installation art has been widely noted to evoke and require the use of the body in various capacities, above and beyond the consideration of formal properties of the art (Reiss, 1999; Best, 2002; Farkhatdinov, 2014; Caldarola, 2020; Vial Kayser and Coëllier, 2021; Kessels and Schrenk, 2022):
First, installation art generally evokes a phenomenological focus on the viewer’s bodily experience and overall body awareness (Bishop, 2005; Petersen, 2015). More specifically, installation art is suggested to be often intended to increase bodily awareness and presence (Oliveira et al., 2003; Bukdahl, 2019). Such conscious experience of one’s body in interaction with the installation is argued to be central to the reception of installation art (Oliveira et al., 2003; Kessels and Schrenk, 2022). For example, in response to Olafur Eliasson’s large-scale installation, The weather project at the Turbine Hall of Tate Modern in 2003 (see Figure 1), which consisted of a large semi-circle of 100 mono-frequency laps, visitors laid down on the floor as if the installation was an actual sun, sensing the heat and brightness of it and observing themselves and others in a huge mirror hung at the ceiling (May and Eliasson, 2003; Bukdahl, 2015). In Olafur Eliason’s installation artwork, Your rainbow panorama (ARoS, 2006–11), visitors could experience what it feels like to walk around inside the installation (Bukdahl, 2019; Ruiz, 2021). Yayoi Kusama’s participatory mirror room installations (e.g., Infinity Mirror Rooms at Tate Modern, 2021–2023) immerse visitors by engulfing their bodies in patterns such that they become one with the artwork (Rosenthal, 2021). Plastique Fantastique’s site-specific installation Blurry Venice at the Venice Biennale in 2019 gave visitors the sensation of walking on a liquid surface in a tunnel while their movements change the shape of the tunnel itself (Myers, 2019). Fujiko Nakaya’s participatory installations use fog (e.g., Nebel Leben at Haus der Kunst, 2022) that envelops visitors allowing for a sensory connection to the fog (Sone, 2019). Bennett (2019) described their whole body feeling floating and weightless while experiencing James Turrell’s Ganzfeld installation Perfectly Clear (1991).
Figure 1. Example installation artworks that were noted to evoke and require the use of the body in various ways.
Second, the participatory audience is expected to play a significant part in generating the work and its reception. The immersive and enveloping character of installation art often achieves this. Such dependence of installation art on the literal presence of the spectators has been reflected since the early 60s under the notion of “inclusion of the viewer” in art historical writings (Rebentisch, 2012, p. 248; see also Hobbs, 2001), according to which the spectator becomes “integral to the completion of the work” (Reiss, 1999, p. xii). Thus, installation art often falls under “relational aesthetics” (Bourriaud et al., 2002), where emphasis is placed on the visitor’s role in the artwork’s realization and reception. Bukdahl (2019) even argued that the appreciation of being participants of the artwork is part of its experience.
Last, the often-large scale and lack of perspicuity (i.e., it cannot be taken in in one glance) of installation artworks require physical activity, active exploration, and spatial movement to be able to fully explore and engage it (Noë, 2000; Bishop, 2005; Manresa, 2020). This becomes especially salient compared to paintings, which could be argued to be taken in, at least for recognition of content and visual properties, more instantaneously. As such, art experience of installation art depends upon bodily investment, which is essential for its experience. It has been argued that corporeal movement during the experience of installation art “activates [the visitors’] physical and psychic responses” rather than the art object alone (Weingarden, 2014, p. 416). Art critics also assigned new importance to this physical mode of experience: “How one saw became as relevant as what one saw” (Mondloch, 2007, p. 23).
2.3. The role of the body for art experience (of installation art)
In turn, on the basis that the body is part of its reception, there are multiple arguments that the body plays a key role in the nature of the experience and the appreciation and evaluation of the art.
Both components are intertwined. Bodily engagement and experience might be considered important elements of aesthetic interaction with artworks that “de facto contribute to our appreciation” (Fingerhut, 2018, p. 84). Best (2002) suggests a constitutive role of the body for the appreciation of installation art in that the body expands the modes of apprehension in addition to more common ways such as vision and the intellect. Furthermore, another important characteristic of installation art, participation, has been argued to be vital for appreciation: “the viewer has to be physically present in the work or a performance of it and has to behave in the prescribed manner while there, so as to enhance his or her appreciation of it” (Novitz, 2001, p. 154). In terms of the participatory role of the visitor discussed above, as opposed to a passive “recipient,” in installation art, the participatory visitor is invited to contemplate their role in the installation, which might lead to a more self-aware and reflective state, and thus heightened art experience (Rebentisch, 2012).
Regarding the role of active movement in appreciation for the experience of architecture, which parallels installation art, it has been argued that the sequence of experiences consequent on movement through a building may be essential to its effect on the viewer. “The feeling of our bodies moving through a building, reaching out and touching its surfaces, listening to its echoes, and so on is a crucial part of appreciation in its own right” (Robinson, 2012, p. 338). Shifting our awareness to our body or general capacity for body awareness could also magnify bodily sensations, which in turn could heighten our aesthetic experience (Montero, 2006; Lanzoni, 2009; Shusterman, 2012). Oliveira et al. (2003) argued that the degree to which our sensory faculties are stimulated in the experience of installation art is linked to the impact that an experience can have on us.
Again, to give an example with architecture, Shusterman (2012) suggested that “If the appreciation of architecture is so strongly linked to somatic experiences [such as proprioception], then heightening somatic consciousness could improve our architectural experience” (p. 14; see Montero, 2006 for a similar argument regarding the enhanced experience of watching and performing dance though shifting awareness to the body). These arguments further amplify that bodily awareness and experience may play an aesthetically relevant role in the receiver’s experience with the addition that more bodily awareness/experiences might heighten art experience, especially of installation art.
2.4. The body and profound or transformative experience
Beyond the proposed role of the body for the appreciation of the artwork, installation art has specifically been argued to cause profound and complex emotional involvements (Bishop, 2005; Vial Kayser and Coëllier, 2021) or even to have transformative potential (Weingarden, 2014, 2015; Sherman and Morrissey, 2017; Yoshitake et al., 2017; Vial Kayser and Coëllier, 2021).
To name a few examples, Carsten Höller said about his participatory installations that “they offer the possibility of unique inner experiences that can be used for the exploration of the self” (Bukdahl, 2015, p. 179, Interview with Carsten Höller by Vincent Honor, op. cit.). The immersive Ganzfeld environments created by artist James Turrell caused visitors’ perception to “become the object of reflection, and led some to a deeper understanding of themselves and their relationship to the external environment, deepening their conception of themselves as embodied beings” (Sherman and Morrissey, 2017, p. 6). Similarly, in discussing his works, Eliasson (2019, p. 19) suggests that increased self-reflection through the awareness of the body and processes of perception “leads to a more reflective attitude toward the world outside the artwork.” By what means people have profound, life-changing experiences within installation art has received increased interest (Pelowski and Akiba, 2011; Weingarden, 2014, 2015; Pelowski et al., 2018; Sadia, 2021).
In fact, a factor that potentially makes the encounters resonant may be the heightened physicality itself. The basis for heightened physicality is the temporal, spatial, and immersive scale of installation artworks, which has been argued to allow reflection on how the spectator feels inside it (Bishop, 2005) or in their presence: “the works make us reflect on how we feel, […], in their presence” (Noë, 2000, p. 131), which in turn facilitate self-awareness and deeper consideration (Pelowski et al., 2018). Body awareness, in general, arguably plays a central role in all subjective experiences (Berlyne, 1971; Djebbara et al., 2019) in that it heightens our awareness of what is around us and ourselves, thereby making us aware of what we are feeling (Brinck, 2018).
One prompt for a switch to an inward reflective mode has been specifically argued to be aware of one’s body. Theoretical writings have drawn the notion of the body/self as a “modality of reflexivity” (Jones, 2000, p. 335), and awareness of the body has been argued to be one of the crucial elements in the process of also becoming aware of oneself (Berlucchi and Aglioti, 2010; Pelowski, 2011; Jelić and Staničić, 2021). In a museum study involving Mark Rothko paintings, which covered all gallery walls in a particularly immersive manner, Pelowski (2015) showed that the body could play an essential role in inducing more self-reflective responses. The author reports that contextual sensations (e.g., feeling watched, hearing one’s footsteps, getting chills from the air conditioning system) may shift attention to one’s body and current actions, especially when facing particularly engrossing or challenging art. This awareness may induce meta-cognitive reflection about one’s experience (Pelowski et al., 2017). Altogether, bodily awareness might function as a crucial trigger for making people more receptive and reflective.
To take the discussion a step further, besides the capacity for general increased body awareness through the spatial character of installation art, installation art often evokes direct and powerful corporeal relations by, e.g., intervening on our balance or posture (Bishop, 2005). By foregrounding certain spatial features or leading/disturbing the visitors’ movement through them, installations serve to force an awareness toward one’s body and responses which were taken for granted in non-art spaces, and “individuals may even come to deeper appreciation by juxtaposing visual and proprioceptive sensations (awareness of being in an [installation])” (Pelowski et al., 2018). Such a specific interaction of interoception and exteroception could also be a hallmark of aesthetic emotions that can lead to more reflective modes of engagement (Fingerhut and Spee, forthcoming).
In architecture, switching visitors’ attention to the body has been successfully employed by architects throughout history to immerse a perceiver into the spatial situation. In moving through space, the body schema is responsible for the continuous tracking of bodily states and positions as we move by integrating interoceptive, proprioceptive, and exteroceptive information and having an awareness of ourselves as embodied beings (Jelić et al., 2016; Jelić and Staničić, 2021). Rupture of this habitual body schema, such as when engaging architecture (or installation art), then activates an attentional switch that allows “the visitor to consciously experience the […] setting and oneself as an experiencing and bodily subject” (p. 7). For example, the architectural design of Carlo Scarpa’s stairs at the Brion Cemetery in San Vito d’Altivole, Italy, is argued to afford each step to be performed with specifically one of but not either the left or right foot, which “ruptures” one’s usual stair-walking behavior to allocate an attentional switch to the conscious experience of one’s body (Jelić et al., 2016). Thus, strong bodily experiences might be crucial triggers to make ourselves more receptive and make the whole experience more profound and meaningful (also see Robinson, 2012, 2021).
2.5. Previous empirical research on installation art
In sum, the above review provides a large number of items that could be related to art experiences with installations and with art, in general, relating to aspects from the basic requirement for engagement with our bodies, to modes of interaction/participation, to various ways of being aware of our bodies and movements, as well as to corresponding or interrelated emotions and cognitive or other responses. Each of these could be treated as potential hypotheses that might individually, or in combination, play a key role in individuals’ reported art experience (see also Table 1, and further discussion in the methods).
Table 1. Twenty-nine self-report body-items (rated on 7-point Likert scales; 1 = Not at all, 7 = A lot).
Looking at past studies on installations, however, only some of the above aspects have been considered. A few studies have started investigating other relevant parts of the engagement and experience of installation art besides the role of subjective bodily experience and awareness. For example, Gulhan et al. (2021) found that gaze behavior inside an installation artwork vs. a virtual reality reproduction did not significantly differ. Another study found that a contemporary critical art exhibition, including installation art along with other mediums, received higher aesthetic appreciation when experienced in a gallery as opposed to a laboratory context (Szubielska et al., 2021), as well as was better understood when a curatorial description was available (Szubielska and Imbir, 2021).
Other studies graphically showed that the spatial arrangement of a sculpture affected visitors’ movements and physiological responses during a gallery visit via maps of movement paths and where on the paths physiological responses (heart rate and skin conductance) were stronger (also see Tröndle and Tschacher, 2012; Tröndle et al., 2014), or assessed more complex emotional/cognitive experiences in combination with viewing behavior (Pelowski et al., 2018). A series of studies started to specifically compare art experience between interactive art installations and non-interactive versions of them, while the authors agree that more research is needed because results on this comparison remain mixed (e.g., Jacucci et al., 2009; Vi et al., 2017; Savaş et al., 2021; Szubielska and Imbir, 2022).
Regarding the body, one study assessed how installation art could have a measurable, objective impact on the body. Kapoula et al. (2014) showed that Richard Serra’s Promenade installation improved visitors’ balance after walking around and alongside the artwork’s laterally tilted monumental elements that play with depth and verticality (see also Nather et al., 2010; Kapoula et al., 2014; Vernet et al., 2018 for examples of how representation of movements or depth in visual art has been shown to modulate posture control). Kapoula et al.’s (2014) study did suggest that spatial properties of installation art can have an objective, measurable impact on the body, and the importance of ecologically valid testing.
However, none of the above studies considered viewers’ subjective experience of the body and how this relates to installation art experience and evaluation. We (Kühnapfel et al., 2023) previously used self-reported bodily items and found that on average participants were generally aware of their body and movement in front of a painting, their distance from and approach to it and that the room/space had influenced the way they encountered the painting while also showing variability in these responses. To our knowledge, only one study with installation art has included assessments of subjective body awareness in a self-report questionnaire. Pelowski et al. (2020) recorded individual’s experiences with three different installation artworks to assess whether they matched what the artists intended. After engaging each installation artwork, participants reported their emotional and phenomenal experience based on a list of 37 self-report items, each rated on an 8-point Likert scale. One item out of these was “awareness of the body”. The mean ratings on “awareness of the body” were among the highest noted ratings in the reports on two of the installation artworks (i.e., second highest rating out of the 37 ratings after “feeling stimulated” for artwork two, and fourth highest out of the 37 ratings after feeling “a sense of confusion,” “stimulated,” and “absorbed” for artwork three). This study suggested that body awareness could be one experience dimension that is rated as considerably high, compared to other emotional and phenomenal experience factors. However, this study did assess only this single item on “awareness of one’s body” and did not relate it to other experience factors, missing to capture or address important nuances of bodily experience and interactions, as well as related to other aspects of art experience.
2.6. Trait awareness of the body and relation to body/art experience
The review so far suggests that relatively more awareness of one’s body also modulates our bodily experiences, which in turn shapes the experience of an artwork. One further question should be who becomes aware of these bodily experiences. Interpersonal differences in awareness of one’s body might translate to different impacts of body awareness or even general responses when engaging in art.
However, to date, no study in empirical aesthetics using visual arts has assessed the role of individual differences in body awareness in art experience. Regarding the experience of dance, researchers found that the ability to accurately detect interoceptive signals (i.e., to count one’s heartbeat in given time windows accurately) modulated the relationship between physiological sensitivity (in terms of Galvanic Skin Response) to dance stimuli varying in expressivity and rated expressivity of the stimuli (Christensen et al., 2021; also see Christensen et al., 2018).
3. Methods
The present study explored participants’ subjective experiences of their body and potential body-related factors, which had been mentioned in the review, and their impact on art experience and evaluation.
3.1. Participants
We involved a final sample of 235 participants (142 female, 89 male, 2 other, 2 preferred not to answer, Mage = 33.82, SD = 14.36, range = 18–72 years). The researchers tried to invite as many museum visitors as possible to participate in the study in the time the museum gave. These individuals constituted a convenience sample of museum visitors. Participants were eligible to participate in the study if they were older than 18 and proficient in English or German. Participants received a museum visit voucher (worth 12 €) as participation compensation. The final sample was derived from an initial collection of 236, with one individual not meeting the participant criteria (under age 18). The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki and the University of Vienna Ethics Committee. A signed informed consent was completed before participation.
3.2. Stimuli
The stimulus was the installation artwork in orbit by Tomás Saraceno (*1973), exhibited at Kunstsammlung Nordrhein-Westphalen (K21) in Düsseldorf, Germany, curated by Marion Ackermann and Susanne Meyer-Bser. The installation (see Figure 2) is composed of a 2,500 m2 (880 m2 walkable area) three-layered net made from 3 mm-diameter stainless-steel cable spanned horizontally at a considerable height of 25 meters over the atrium of the K21, upon which visitors could enter and explore the different interconnected layers of the net or sit/lay while suspended in space. The net’s three levels were spread apart from each other by five air-inflated PVC (3.5–8 m ø) shells in the form of spheres made of PVC. The installation was part of a larger project called “Cloud Cities,” which addresses the ideas of flying cities or living clouds (Ackermann et al., 2011). Like a spider/web, visitors can feel the movements of others in the net, echoing Saraceno’s artistic practice and research on hybrid and interspecies forms of communication (Kunstsammlung NRW, 2013). Visitors had to bring their own sturdy footwear (e.g., hiking boots, no sneakers) or borrow suitable footwear from the museum. The museum also provided overalls to wear on top of one’s clothes as protection. Visitors were not allowed to bring any objects into the installation, had to take off jewelry or watches, and wear an FFP2 mask (due to COVID-19 protocols).
Figure 2. Tomás Saraceno—in orbit, installation view K21, Düsseldorf, Germany. Tomás Saraceno—in orbit, 2013,-ongoing, installation view K21, ©Tomás Saraceno. Grateful acknowledgment is given for written permission by Studio Tomás Saraceno to reproduce this photograph.
This installation artwork was chosen because it matched several of the above-discussed aspects, such as audience participation, bodily engagement, and awareness of one’s body in general and various bodily experiences, which are argued to be integral to the reception of this installation, as well as its connection to profound/self-reflective experiences. The installation in orbit is participatory in that visitors must enter it to experience it fully (Putnam, 2014). The role of the participatory audience in completing the work especially becomes apparent in this statement on the installation by artist Tomás Saraceno: “If there is no person in the work, you don’t see the work. It is invisible.” (Kunstsammlung NRW, 2013).
According to anecdotal reports, visitors routinely report experiences related to the body in terms of proprioceptive (e.g., awareness of one’s movement or sensing tension of one’s muscles) and interoceptive sensations (e.g., awareness of one’s breathing or heart pounding) (Putnam, 2014; Hanz, 2019; Wattolik, 2019; Kessels and Schrenk, 2022; Spence, 2022). in orbit also provides a perfect example of how bodily experience is triggered to make the art encounter resonant (Jelić et al., 2016): as visitors enter the participatory installation, their bodily experience is stimulated or even ruptured in terms of altering visitors’ posture, balance, or perception of body weight (Frank, 2013; Trailer: Tomás Saraceno—In orbit, 2013).
Engagement with the installation also offers profound/self-reflective experiences as reported in anecdotal reports and interviews with the artist. In the exhibition trailer, the artist, Tomás Saraceno, describes his intentions: “I want to provoke feelings, make people more sensitive to the installation, other people, or even the weather. And who knows, maybe this sensitivity will lead to more peace. […] I am interested in how this installation generates new ideas” (Trailer: Tomás Saraceno—In Orbit, 2013). The text about the installation on the museum website states the following intentions: “In orbit is a methodological attempt to achieve an ethical sensitivity, becoming aware of the many phenomena that shape our possibilities of being-in-the-world and our role in it, making them perceivable through a unique synesthetic experience.” Visitors have anecdotally reported such experiences of self-reflection and perspective change (e.g., Chin, 2013; Divisare, 2013), as well as other thought processes, argued to be triggered by the environment created by the artist (Prinsi, 2013).
3.3. Procedure
Data were collected over 10 days from the 9th to the 19th of March 2022 (11:00 a.m. to 5:30 p.m.). This ending time allowed us to stop collection before sunset so that the presence of daylight and/or artificial lights was relatively consistent for all participants. A maximum of seven people were allowed to enter the installation for approximately 10 min for safety reasons.
We employed a collection design whereby participants were stopped by a researcher immediately after they had exited the installation and asked to participate. This allowed us to assess the experiences spontaneously had by participants without prior priming or fore-knowledge that individuals would be participating in a study.
Participants were asked to read and sign the informed consent if they agreed. The survey was administered via the online commercial survey tool Qualtrics (Qualtrics Int., Seattle, WA, USA). It could be accessed via a QR code, allowing participants to answer questions on their own smartphones or via tablet computers provided by the researcher. Participants could take the survey in German or English. The survey lasted about 20 min. After the experiment, participants were debriefed about the purpose of the study and handed the museum ticket voucher as compensation.
3.4. Measures 3.4.1. Bodily experience
The main aim of the survey was to collect participant reports regarding a range of reactions tied to awareness of the body (or, alternatively, to assess whether and to what extent such factors were noted by participants as part of their experience). This was accomplished in two ways—a survey (quantitative) based on the above review of theoretical and anecdotal arguments and an open-ended free answer (qualitative assessment).
3.4.1.1. Quantitative survey
Participants reported their subjective feelings or awareness of body-items using a list of 29 factors created by our research team, which are summarized in Table 1. The items were based on the above review regarding the possible way the body might be involved in the experience of installation art and with the further aim of later assessing the items’ importance and interrelation using network science. The items captured the participation/feeling part of the artwork, inner (e.g., heartbeat) or outer (e.g., chills) bodily sensations, awareness of the space/visitors around one and one’s existence in/relation to it (e.g., groundedness, immersion). We also included some contextual sensations that have been argued to trigger awareness of one’s body (e.g., feeling the movements of others or a sense of being watched) (Pelowski et al., 2014), as well as rather negatively balanced experience aspects that could components of art experience in real-world settings (e.g., feeling exhausted, or disoriented) (Pelowski and Akiba, 2011; Pelowski et al., 2014).
Items were introduced with the instruction, “Please think about your art experience. While engaging with the art, I felt…” (e.g., “my body,” “my breathing,” and “my heartbeat”), which participants rated on continuous 7-point Likert-type scales ranging from 1 = “Not at all” to 7 = “A lot.” The 29 items were displayed as two lists, to split up the body item list into two pages. The first list included the first 17 items of Table 1, and the second list included the last 12 items. Item order was randomized within each list. To assess whether participants enjoyed their bodily experience, we also asked the question, “How much did you enjoy your overall bodily experience?” on a 7-point Likert-type scale (1 = “Not at all” to 7 = “A lot”).
3.4.1.2. Qualitative assessment
We also invited participants to reflect upon and report experiences related to their body (if any) via filling out an open text box. This assessment was placed before the body-item list to allow us to assess the spontaneous subjective reports independent of possible priming effects from our scale-based questions. We believe that it is important that some associative, qualitative analysis should complement the quantitative survey/network model. This provides us with more vivid, subjective reports that can be used to interpret the resulting network communities (i.e., how the experience in each community might have felt) and a bottom-up way to discover factors not anticipated in our review of relevant body-items. The qualitative data analysis was performed before the network analysis such that the researchers were not biased by it.
3.4.2. Art experience
We included additional questions to assess other factors relating to art evaluation or notable emotional/transformative responses to the art. To measure art evaluation, we asked how much participants “liked” the installation, as well as how “meaningful” and how “interesting” they found it. We focused on these three appraisals as they were consistently noted in the review above about bodily experience and are part of common in empirical aesthetics assessments (e.g., Brieber et al., 2015; Tinio and Gartus, 2018; Specker et al., 2021; Kühnapfel et al., 2023), but also assessed a series of other potentially relevant appraisal factors that are reported with the descriptives and correlations to each other in Supplementary Table 2 but were not focus for the present analysis. One of those items was whether participants assessed the installation as “good” art, which is underrepresented in empirical aesthetics (Fingerhut and Prinz, 2018) and will be addressed in future studies.
To capture potential self-reflective and perspective-changing experiences, we asked for “self-reflection” and “gaining a new perspective,” which are associated with a transformative/schema change outcome (Pelowski et al., 2017, also see Pelowski and Akiba, 2011). In addition, as described in the stimuli section, perspective change and self-reflection were experiences noted explicitly by the artist and anecdotal reports. Finally, we also asked whether participants experienced “a transformation” to directly capture the potential transformative character of installation art in general in a subjective way (Weingarden, 2014, 2015; Sherman and Morrissey, 2017; Yoshitake et al., 2017; Vial Kayser and Coëllier, 2021). All items were rated on 7-point Likert scales ranging from 1 = “Not at all” to 7 = “A lot.”
In addition, we included two specific emotional terms regarding how much participants experienced “awe” and “wonder.” These were selected because they are often noted as salient feelings regarding particularly profound aesthetic or art experiences (Fingerhut, 2018; Fingerhut and Prinz, 2018). In addition, awe and wonder have been argued to play a role in stimulating new ways of thinking and understanding (Pelowski et al., 2017), schema/knowledge change (Keltner and Haidt, 2003; Shiota et al., 2007), personal meaning, transformation, and profound experience (Schneider, 2009; Cohen et al., 2010; Nusbaum and Silvia, 2014), and are connected to self-awareness (Yaden et al., 2017). These two terms were also mentioned anecdotally in conjunction with in orbit in online articles (e.g., Jones, 2013; Boehling, 2016).
3.4.3. Individual differences in trait body awareness
We employed the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness Version 2 (MAIA-2; Mehling et al., 2018) to assess individual differences in interoceptive bodily awareness. The MAIA-2 is a validated and established state-trait questionnaire to measure multiple dimensions of interoception by self-report. It consists of 37 statements, which participants rate on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “Never” to “Always.” An overall score is calculated by summing and averaging all items, which was also the main assessment for the present study. This total ranges from 0 to 5, with higher scores reflecting greater interoceptive sensitivity. The original MAIA-2 questionnaire was used in the English version of the survey, and the German version of the MAIA-2 was used in the German survey (for the validated German version see Bornemann et al., 2015; Eggart et al., 2021).
In addition, the battery can also be divided into eight subscales: (1) “Noticing” changes in one’s body when being uncomfortable (e.g., I notice when I am uncomfortable in my body), (2) “Not-Distracting,” or the tendency to ignore or distract oneself from sensations of pain or discomfort (e.g., I distract myself from sensations of discomfort), (3) “Not-Worrying,” emotional distress or worry with sensations of pain or discomfort (e.g., When I am in discomfort or pain I can’t get it out of my mind), (4) “Attention Regulation,” ability to remain focused on the body even when distracted (e.g., I can return awareness to my body if I am distracted), (5) “Emotional Awareness,” ability to notice changes in one’s body when feeling a certain way (e.g., I notice that my breathing becomes free and easy when I feel comfortable), (6) “Self-Regulation,” one’s ability to use focus on one’s body to reduce stress or tension (e.g., I can use my breath to reduce tension), (7) “Body Listening,” using one’s body to learn about one’s emotional state (I listen to my body to inform me), and (8) “Trust,” seeing one’s body as a safe place and trusting one’s sensations (I trust my body sensations).
3.4.4. Demographics, familiarity, and social experience
Finally, we asked several questions to collect more contextual factors that might have influenced experiences or reports. These included: familiarity, assessed by asking whether participants had experienced the installation before (excluding the time just before the survey) and, if yes, how many times. We also asked participants whether they experienced the installation alone or with others (e.g., friends, family, colleagues), following studies suggesting that experiencing art together vs. alone can influence the art experience (Pelowski et al., 2014 for review). Art engagement, in terms of frequency of museum visits (pre–COVID-19 crisis), art education, fear of height, art preference, and demographical data (age and gender), was also recorded.
4. Results
An overview of the participant demographics and other characteristics can be seen in Supplementary Table 1. Overall, our participant sample was generally interested in art (M = 6.23, SD = 1.63; on a 7-point scale). However, 78.7% responded that they had no prior art education/knowledge. For 83.4% of the sample, it was the first time they had encountered the installation, while the remaining 11.1% had visited an average of 1.5 times before (range = 1–3 previous visits). Most participants (83.4%) went to the installation with friends/family/colleagues. The average score on the fear of heights rating was on the positive side of the scale (M = 4.39, SD = 2.19; out of 7 points), with just over half of the participants (52%) scoring on the positive side (ratings 5, 6, 7) but with 45% suggesting relatively high agreement (6–7 points).
Our sample scored moderately high in the overall MAIA-2 score, with the highest distinctiveness in the “Emotional Awareness” subscale, which indicates the ability to notice changes in one’s body when feeling a certain way, and the lowest in the “Not-Distracting” subscale, which indicates the tendency to ignore or distract oneself from sensations of pain or discomfort (see Supplementary Table 1). In the current sample, Cronbach’s α indices of internal consistency reliability of the MAIA-2 was 0.71 across the eight scales, which is in the middle of typical ranges of 0.64 to 0.83 (Mehling et al., 2018).
4.1. Descriptive results: art appraisal and emotional experience
For an overview of the descriptive statistics of the dataset, see Table 2. We found a wide range of responses for most of the appraisal scales, with means generally above the midpoint, indicating that participants found the artwork relatively interesting (M = 4.37) and meaningful (M = 4.61) and particularly liked the artwork (M = 6.03). The reported experiences of Awe and Perspective change were also positive. For Transformation (M = 3.29), Self-reflection (M = 3.87), and especially Wonder, did we find notably lower average ratings (M = 1.77) with suggestions of a floor effect. Participants generally enjoyed their overall bodily experience (M = 5.51).
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of artwork ratings.
4.2. Bodily experience
Descriptive statistics of the means and CI intervals of the different scale-based answers to the body-items are shown in Figure 3. Note that we excluded the item “absorbed” because, during data collection, many German-speaking participants indicated that they did not understand what the term meant in exit interviews, resulting in 28 items. As can be seen, the mean for most scales tended to fall around the midpoint of most scales, with a rather high variance across the participants, and, notably, little indication of major skew or ceiling/floor effects.
Figure 3. Graph showing means and 95% confidence intervals for the 28 body-items. Note, communities are based on later network analysis.
The highest noted terms were awareness of movement, balance, participants’ overall body, and temperature. The least noted items were feeling grounded, disoriented, and immersed, indicating that participants reported mostly experiences directly related to the awareness of their own body rather than how their body specifically felt or how it felt in relation to the net/other visitors.
4.2.1. Scalar data reduction, network analysis
To clarify the underlying psychological dimensions of bodily responses and their relationships to the experience of the installation art piece, we then used techniques from network science. The application of network science in empirical aesthetics has become more popular in recent years as a tool to discover underlying latent structures in multivariate data (e.g., Pelowski et al., 2018; Coburn et al., 2020; Hayn-Leichsenring et al., 2020; Specker et al., 2021; Weinberger et al., 2021; Christensen et al., 2022). This approach consisted of network construction, followed by community identification and stability analyses. The following analyses were conducted using the R software (R Core Team, 2022), and the script is available at the Open Science Framework (OSF) https://osf.io/tgebw/.
4.2.1.1. Network construction
To prepare the body-item dataset, five participants were removed as multivariate outliers via Mahalanobis Distances larger than the critical chi-square value for df s at alpha = 0.001 (Mahalanobis, 1936), resulting in a sample of N = 230 (140 female, 87 male, one other, two preferred not to answer, Mage = 33.96, SD = 14.40, range = 18–72 years) with N = 28 body-items. We used the Triangulated Maximally Filtered Graph (TMFG; see Massara et al., 2015) to construct the network using the NetworkToolbox package (Christensen, 2018) in R (see Christensen et al., 2019, 2020; Pelowski et al., 2021 for similar applications). The TMFG does not assume that data are multivariate normal (Golino et al., 2020). Details on the algorithm are in the Supplementary material, and the final TMFG network is in Supplementary Figure 1.
4.2.2. Community identification
We used Exploratory Graph Analysis (EGA; Golino and Demetriou, 2017; Golino and Epskamp, 2017) to determine the number of dimensions in the data (i.e., identification of communities of related items) (Golino and Epskamp, 2017), using the used R packages EGAnet (version 1.1.0; Golino et al., 2022) and qgraph (Epskamp et al., 2012). Like principal component or factor analysis, this approach offers data reduction abilities while, in addition providing information about centrality, interconnection, and relative importance of items, allowing for the visualization of the entire network. EGA uses network psychometrics and has been shown to be less affected by aspects such as unidimensionality, interfactor correlations, and sample size, as well as to be more accurate than traditional methods of dimension reduction and factor analysis (e.g., Scee test, parallel analysis, K1 rule; Golino and Demetriou, 2017; Golino and Epskamp, 2017; Golino et al., 2020). This is followed by “community” or dimension identification in the network. For this, we applied the walktrap algorithm via the igraph package (Csárdi and Nepusz, 2006) in R, which uses “random walks” or a certain number of random “steps” or from one node to another. The algorithm is deterministic and data-driven (i.e., communities are discovered without the researcher’s guidance (Christensen and Golino, 2021). Analyses on structural consistency are reported in Supplementary material.
4.2.3. Network model results
The final network is shown in Figure 4. Nodes, indicated by the colored and numbered circles, represent the measurement items. Nodes are connected by unidirectional edges which represent associations between items (i.e., zero-order correlation surviving the TMFG algorithm). Blue lines indicate positive relations, red lines indicate negative, and thickness indicates the strength of relations.
Figure 4. Exploratory graph analysis (EGA) dimensional structure of the body items.
Network loadings, computed via the net.loads function from the EGAnet package, which computes the between- and within-community strength of each item for each community, are shown in Table 3 (highest loadings in bold). The network identified four communities (discussed in descending order, starting with the highest loading items printed in bold per community): (1) a community of four items that generally represented “Presence” (i.e., felt presence either within one’s own body or feeling a more out-of-body, transcendent state), including feeling “weightless,” “grounded,” “revived,” and “part of the art” (mean of the items of Community 1 = 4.14, SD = 1.99); (2) eight items that represented “Proprioceptive experiences”—awareness of the “body,” “movement,” “existence,” “space,” “posture,” “balance,” “weight,” “core,” and “gravity” (M = 4.85, SD = 1.77); (3) 11 items that described “Disturbed experiences”—feeling “awkward,” “watched,” “chills,” “vertigo,” “exhausted,” “disorientated,” “shaking,” “immersed,” “unable to move,” the “movement of others,” and being about to “fall” (M = 3.32, SD = 1.95); and (4) 4 items represented “Interoceptive experiences”—feeling one’s “sweat,” “temperature,” “heart beating,” and “breathing” (M = 4.47, SD = 1.9). Note that, as with principal component analysis, the naming of components requires some degree of interpretation from the researchers, and thus the bolded labels represent our own best attempt to provide a general label to the collection of items.
Table 3. Exploratory graph analysis (EGA) network loadings per community.
4.3. Relationship between body experience communities and art experience
For subsequent analysis, we then computed network scores per participant to represent each individual’s placement in each community, based on each node’s strength within each Community (i.e., factor) in the network using the net.scores function from the EGAnet package. These values are used as network “factor loadings” for the weights of each item (Christensen and Golino, 2021). Notably, network analysis allows nodes to contribute to more than one community, i.e., the network scores account for cross-loadings in their estimation of scores. These loadings are considered in the network scores.
To assess the role that the communities of bodily experiences had on art experience, we then used the participants’ network scores in multiple regressions to predict specific art experiences (Awe, Wonder, Interest, Liking, Meaning, Self-reflection, Perspective Change, and Transformation) (see Table 4 for following results). There was no substantive multi-collinearity in any of the models (all VIFs were < 3.70).
Table 4. Summary of regression analysis for art experience.
Regarding the emotional experiences among them, the combined model significantly predicted Awe [F(4, 225) = 8.95, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.13, Radjusted2 = 0.12]. Specifically, high scorings on Community 3 (Disturbance) significantly predicted higher Awe (t = 3.00, p = 0.002). The ratings on Wonder had a strong floor effect, with more than half of the participant sample (N = 116) not experiencing any wonder, i.e., rating “1” on the Likert scale. Only 11.3% of the sample gave Wonder experience ratings above the midpoint of four (N = 26). Given this extreme pattern of ratings and a small sample of participant rating it above the midpoint, we did not compute the regression with the Wonder ratings.
The art appraisals, Liking [F(4, 224) = 10.66, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.14, Radjusted2 = 0.12], Meaning [F(4, 223) = 8.69, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.14, Radjusted2 = 0.12], and Interest [F(4, 225) = 11.31, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.17, Radjusted2 = 0.15], were significantly predicted by the combined model. Liking (t = 3.47, p = 0.001) and Meaning (t = 3.13, p = 0.003) were significantly predicted by higher scorings on Community 2 (Proprioception). Interest was also significantly predicted by high scorings on Community 2 (Proprioception) (t = 4.05, p < 0.001), but also Community 4 (Interoception) (t = 2.39, p = 0.018).
Finally, we ran two multiple regressions looking at the self-reflective/transformative experience factors. We found a collective significant effect between the four communities and Change in Perspective [F(4, 225) = 10.6, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.16, Radjusted2 = 0.14]. Higher scorings on Community 2 (Proprioception) significantly predicted higher Perspective changing experiences (t = 4.27, p < 0.001). Self-reflection [F(4, 225) = 8.48, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.13, Radjusted2 = 0.12] was predicted by Community 2 (Proprioception) (t = .83, p = 0.005) and Community 3 (Disturbance) (t = 2.12, p = 0.035). In the same way, Transformation [F(4,225) = 15.11, p = 0.001, R2 = 0.21, Radjusted2 = 0.20] was predicted by Community 2 (Proprioception) (t = 5.64, p < 0.001) and Community 3 (Disturbance) (t = 3.47, p = 0.001).
4.4. Enjoyment of bodily experience and relation to communities and art experience
The combined model assessing the impact of the network dimensions on the enjoyment of the overall bodily experience was also significant [F(4, 225) = 0.71, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.42, Radjusted2 = 0.41] (see Supplementary Table 3 for full results). Specifically, enjoyment of the bodily experience was predicted by Community 2 (Proprioception; t = 5.53, p < 0.001), Community 3 (Disturbance; t = −3.01, p = 0.003), and Community 4 (Interoception; t = 2.02, p = 0.045). These results indicated that participants enjoyed interoceptive and proprioceptive experiences, but not the experiences that were grouped as disturbing.
To explore the relationship between the enjoyment of the overall bodily experience and art experience, we ran correlations between them [with a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of 0.008 per test (0.05/6)]. We found a significant positive correlation between overall bodily enjoyment and Liking, rP(221) = 0.41, p < 0.001, 95% CI of correlation (0.29, 0.51), Meaning, rP(220) = 0.37, p < 0.001, 95% CI of correlation (0.25, 0.47), Interest, rP(222) = 0.24, p = 0.001, 95% CI of correlation (0.12, 0.36), Self-Reflection, rP(221) = 0.16, p = 0.022, 95% CI of correlation (0.03, 0.28), Perspective change, rP(221) = 0.29, p < 0.001, 95% CI of correlation (0.17, 0.40), and Transformation, rP(233) = 0.23, p < 0.001, 95% CI of correlation (0.11, 0.35), indicating that participants who enjoyed the bodily experience also liked the art more, found it more meaningful and interesting, reported that it made them reflect about themselves and their perspectives and that their experience was transformative.
4.5. The role of trait interoceptive bodily awareness in art experience 4.5.1. Relationship between trait interoceptive bodily awareness and body experience communities
We then focused on the relation between trait interoceptive bodily awareness (the total score of the MAIA-2 questionnaire) and the reported bodily experience. Simple linear regression analysis indicated that participants who were higher in interoceptive bodily awareness (total MAIA-2 score) scored lower on Community 3 (Disturbance) [F(1, 225) = 5.04, p = 0.026, R2 = 0.02, Radjusted2 = 0.12]. Interoceptive bodily awareness did not affect the other three communities.
For exploratory purposes, to follow-up on the effect of the total interoceptive bodily awareness score (MAIA-2) on scorings in Community 3 (Disturbance), we further examined the relationship between the eight subscales of the MAIA-2 and Community 3 (Disturbance). The MAIA-2 subscales allow capturing finer dimensions of interoception (see Methods for the description of each subscale). The adjusted alpha level following the Bonferroni correction was set to 0.006 (0.05/8). Community 3 (Disturbance) positively correlated “Body Trusting” subscale, rP(228) = −0.31, p = 0.001, 95% CI of correlation (−0.32, −0.08), which indicates seeing one’s body as a safe place and trusting one’s sensations. All other subscales showed a non-significant correlation to Community 3 (Disturbance). This result indicated that participants who tended to experience their body as safe and trustworthy to a lesser degree have experiences associated with disturbing experiences [i.e., high scorings on Community 3 (Disturbance)].
4.5.2. Role of trait interoceptive bodily awareness in the relationship between art experience and communities
Next, we assessed whether relatively more awareness of one’s body (MAIA-2) might modulate how we attend to bodily experience and in turn, magnify art appreciation (Jung et al., 2017; Brinck, 2018; Schino et al., 2021). To address this question, we investigated whether MAIA-2 moderated the relationship between the four Communities and art appraisal (Liking, Interest, and Meaning) by adding body awareness as an interaction term with the Communities to the regression models predicting appraisal. Interoception did not moderate the relationship between the four Communities and the appraisal ratings (see Supplementary Table 4 for full results).
4.6. What do visitors spontaneously report about bodily experiences with installation art?
As a last step, we looked at the open-ended answers to further address our first research question regarding what bodily experiences visitors report.
4.6.1. Qualitative content analysis
A total of 235 qualitative records from the final participant sample were obtained (see descriptive demographic statistics in Supplementary Table 1), with 36 records not used for the analysis because responses did not mention experiences related to the body, which was specially asked for in the open question, or participants responded with only a single word, leaving a sample of N = 199 for the qualitative analysis. Each response included, on average two mentions of experiences related to the body. Response data were analyzed in a bottom-up manner using inductive content analysis (ICA), a form of qualitative content analysis (Thomas, 2006; Bengtsson, 2016; Kyngäs, 2020; Bingham and Witkowsky, 2022).
Table 5 shows the final 12 categories of bodily experiences reported by the visitors. The semantic categories are depicted in decreasing order of occurrence in the descriptions collected.
Table 5. Coding scheme to categorize visitors’ bodily experience of the installation, characteristic terms, and frequency.
4.6.1.1. Example written responses for communities
To connect the results of our qualitative analysis with our quantitative analysis, we categorized the qualitative categories into the four Communities from the network analysis where applicable (see row “Community” in Table 5) after we had analyzed the quantitative data. For these communities, written examples of what the experience type based on the community might have felt like for the participants are given in the last column. Three categories (body trusting, new/heightened body experience, and awareness of the body in relation to the net) were not explicitly captured by our communities. Thus, we specifically focus on these categories in the following.
Many participants reflected on their trust in and abilities of their body (N = 39, labeled “Body Trusting”), with many reporting that they were able to overcome physical limitations or challenges over time and by trusting their own bodies. Based on the exploration of the MAIA-2 subscales in our previous analysis, we assume that participants who tended to experience their bodies as safe and trustworthy tend to have a more positive and less disturbing/distracting art experience.
Eleven participants reported that they had new or intensified bodily experiences, e.g., “I felt aware of my body in a way I didn’t before”. While we do assume that the body trusting category was rather largely based on the nature of the case installation artwork, we would suggest including an assessment on whether visitors experience new or heightened awareness/feelings of their body in future assessments of installation art, which he had not been captured in our items but seems to be an important aspect, which has been described with other installation artworks. For example, Kessels and Schrenk (2022) discuss the installation Tight Roaring Circle (1997) by Dana Caspersens, William Forsythes, and Joel Ryans, which consists of a giant white bouncy castle at the Roundhouse London. Once entering the bouncy castle, the most natural movements become mannered and thus palpable: the installation gives visitors the opportunity to experience new proprioceptive and interoceptive sensations.
Interestingly, a fair number of participants (N = 24) reflected upon their physical relation to the net, such as how it responds to one’s moving in terms of giving in to weight and shaking or how one’s body size fits in (e.g., “One is aware of one’s size and shape in relation to the net. How one sets oneself apart, intervenes, fits in”). One individual specifically reported that they enjoyed feeling the vibrations while lying on the net (“I like that I can feel the vibrations of others while lying down”). This experience type would be mostly related to feeling a “part of the art,” which was part of Community 1 (Presence). However, besides the participatory aspect, we want to emphasize that this category summarizes an important aspect of the experience of installation art that we did not capture in our review above yet, which is that visitors engage with and are aware of the physical properties of the artwork in relation to their own bodies. This illustrates that installation art is not viewed from a distance but is always experienced in relation to one’s body and interaction with it, which should be included in future assessments of installation art.
5. Discussion
This study outlined the ways the body is addressed in installation art and discussed its role in the appreciation of such artworks. Overall, we identified a significant need to consider and empirically assess the role of the body in the experience of installation art. We aimed to fill that gap by capturing visitors’ subjective bodily experiences in an installation art piece (Tomás Saraceno’s installation in orbit) using a mixed-methods design (qualitative content analysis and network analysis of quantitative data) to explore the relationship between bodily experience, individual differences in body awareness, and art experience.
We chose 28 body experience items to capture which kinds of subjective bodily experiences individuals might report and could group them into four communities using a network modeling approach to illustrate their connections. Two communities entailed body-items related more to bodily positions and movements of the body vs. inner aspects. Experiences related to sensing one’s outer body and movements were categorized as Proprioception (Community 2) (i.e., perception of muscle tensions, movement, posture, and balance), while experiences related to sensing one’s inner body were termed Interoception (Community 4) (i.e., perception of sensations from inside the body related to internal organ function, such as heartbeat and respiration; Mehling et al., 2009). Community 3 (Disturbance) involved items that might especially cause a disturbance or rupture of one’s bodily schema in terms of, e.g., stability and orientation, potentially allocating an attentional switch to the conscious experience of one’s body while being in the installation (Jelić et al., 2016). Finally, Community 1 (Presence) reflected experiences capturing the important element of participation and how installation art can make one aware of one’s existence and presence in the space (Noë, 2000; Bishop, 2005).
Regarding the role of the communities in the art experience, we found that especially Proprioception (Community 2) played an important role in the evaluation of the art (Interest, Liking, and Meaning) and transformative outcomes (Self-reflection, Perspective Change, and Transformation). Thus, we recommend that future research in installation art should assess experiences related to Community 2 Proprioception as it shows relations to art appreciation. Using our suggestion, future studies ought to test the hypothesis that more reported proprioceptive experiences, above other experiences, lead to more intense and impactful art experience, as suggested in our theoretical and anecdotal review (Best, 2002; Bishop, 2005; Montero, 2006; Lanzoni, 2009; Rebentisch, 2012; Shusterman, 2012).
Interestingly, Community 3 Disturbance predicted Transformation. This finding supports theoretical arguments that suggest a key role of disruption within the process of transformation in art experience (Pelowski et al., 2017; Kühnapfel and Fingerhut, forthcoming). In this line, we also found that Community 3 Disturbance predicted Awe, an emotion that had been specifically associated with self-relevance and transformation (Keltner and Haidt, 2003; Shiota et al., 2007; Schneider, 2009; Cohen et al., 2010; Silvia and Nusbaum, 2011; Yaden et al., 2017; Fingerhut and Prinz, 2018, 2020). Transformation may especially occur in situations where there is a new relation to the environment and routines become disturbed and not fluent. Indeed, the kind of movement the installation affords in the net can be in such a way function as a bodily disturbance. Furthermore, certain “triggers”, such as feeling chills or being watched, can shift the visitors’ attention to the self, inducing reflection (Pelowski, 2015). In addition, a kind of physical intervention or even rupture of our body in the installation (Jelić et al., 2016; Jelić and Staničić, 2021) might be central for a deeper awareness during art engagement and thus for transformative outcomes. It might be that, after all that, the way installation addresses the body “interrupts, challenges and engages us in a way that is directed at something beyond the ordinary” and induces a “perspectival change that we value in art” (Fingerhut, 2018, p. 87).
All four communities based on the network analysis were also captured in the open-ended questions, indicating that they are part of the art experience. Thus, future studies assessing the art experience of installation art should at least assess some items pertaining to all four communities to capture a broad range, as well as more disturbing or negative experiences (see Community 3 Disturbance) if interested in transformative outcomes. In addition, we suggest assessing experiences related to new or heightened bodily experience and how one feels in relation to the artwork and space, which were revealed to be essential experience aspects via the complementary bottom-up qualitative but not directly captured in our top-down body items/communities.
Regarding emotional experience, surprisingly, we found a floor effect of wonder ratings. It has been argued that many emotions, including wonder, have specific embodiment or somatic profiles (Fingerhut and Prinz, 2018, 2020). Wonder, as examined in studies on emotional faces (e.g., Feleky, 1914) or depictions of wonder in artworks (e.g., Self-Portrait by Franz Xaver Messerschmidt, 1976), may yield a slack jaw, widening of eyes, a slight lift of brows, or a head lift. To feel wonder, one might thus have to remain rather static and transfixed (Fingerhut and Prinz, 2018) to save processing resources for the high perceptual and cognitive engagement that are typical of wonder (also see “stopping for knowledge” and similar arguments (Sarasso et al., 2020; Fingerhut and Kühnapfel, forthcoming). Similarly, it has been argued that high body awareness may also be contrary to absorbing, harmonious, and wonderous experiences (Pelowski and Akiba, 2011). One fascinating outcome of our study was that we found a possible dissociation of awe from wonder that is often treated as similar in the literature. While participants gave overall very low ratings for wonder (M = 1.77), the awe ratings were relatively high (M = 4.52). As we have argued, wonder might have a more cognitive sub-emotional component than the more straightforward, overwhelming awe (Fingerhut and Prinz, 2018). This form of cognitive complexity or challenge might lack from our installation art piece constitutes a rather bodily challenge. Similar things might be said regarding the spiritual sub-emotional component of wonder: veneration might not relate as well to the playful engagement required to engage Saraceno’s work. Future research with installation art needs to refine this relationship between bodily involvements and wonder experiences.
We assessed whether participants who generally attend to their body more than others (as measured by the MAIA-2) report more/specific bodily experiences. We found that the body trusting subscale drove the only found effect of the MAIA-2 on Community 3 (Disturbance). This indicates that participants who generally feel less safe in their bodies also rated the Disturbance items higher. We note that this effect is very specific to the installation, which makes visitors deal with the fear of height and trusting their body in the net, which suggests that individual differences in the MAIA-2 might not be related to overall bodily art experience other than those related to height and insecurity characteristics of the case installation. This effect also fits our finding that participants who enjoyed their bodily experience reported higher liking and meaning, indicating a relationship between enjoying/appreciating one’s bodily experience and art evaluation. Furthermore, this suggests that body awareness/experience that is uncomfortable or not enjoyed might also have a negative outcome, as has been argued previously: if individuals feel rather uncomfortable, unsure, or insecure in a situation, this might be enhanced by body awareness and lead to reduced attention on the artwork (Pelowski et al., 2014).
We also found no moderation of the MAIA-2 score on the relationship between the communities and art experience, also suggesting no role of individual differences in body awareness for art experience. This null result might also be because the MAIA-2 is mainly concerned with inner bodily sensations. Nevertheless, we chose MAIA-2 to be the best fit for assessing body awareness among available body awareness trait questionnaires, which focus on the awareness of internal bodily sensations (Miller et al., 1981), sensitivity to bodily processes (Shields et al., 1989), subjective experiences of organs and the autonomic nervous system (Porges, 1993), or the tendency to integrate body sensations into conscious awareness to guide decision making and behavior (Daubenmier, 2005, also see Three-domain Interoceptive Sensations Questionnaire, Vlemincx et al., 2021; Interoceptive Confusion Questionnaire, Brewer et al., 2016). This also calls for a validated survey assessing general body awareness, also covering proprioceptive, kinesthetic, and exteroceptive dimensions besides interoceptive ones. To summarize, whether the appreciation of the bodily experience and bodily participation in the artwork really is an integral part of the art evaluation and assessment of its quality needs to be addressed in future studies using to-be-developed general trait body awareness surveys.
6. Caveats, implications for future research, and conclusion
This study has limitations. We aimed to conduct and analyze a unique case study, and our presented results, therefore, cannot generalize to other artworks. However, we are confident that due to our rather large participant sample, we were able to cover a good range of variety in responses.
Thus, future analyses should include a broader range of installation art. We also hope that future studies will formulate more focused hypotheses, such as, e.g., increased body awareness, and relatively more bodily experience could make the installation art encounter more intense, resonant, or memorable, as was suggested in the theoretical literature (e.g., Best, 2002; Oliveira et al., 2003; Robinson, 2012) and based on our finding of the positive association of art experience outcomes (i.e., liking, meaning, and interest) and bodily experiences related to proprioception. Future studies could also assess whether bodily experience differs based on participants’ art interest and knowledge levels, which have been shown to impact aesthetic experience with other art forms (see Specker et al., 2020 for review). A unified and validated body experience survey is needed to compare findings from different installation artworks. A further complication comes with the field of embodied aesthetic emotions (such as wonder and interest): engagement and evaluative elements share embodied resources (Fingerhut and Kühnapfel, forthcoming). This can make it hard to identify which bodily responses are necessitated by the artwork (to experience it at all) and which contribute to an embodied evaluation of the artwork. This problem more directly might pertain to installation art.
While our study was survey-based, future studies could also add recordings of the physiological data (e.g., see suggestions in Schino et al., 2022) and track participants’ amount of movement (e.g., Kühnapfel et al., 2023) to capture more objective indicators of physical engagement, followed by subjective reports to get insight how specific engagement might have felt.
Future studies could also employ installation artworks that do not evoke such direct and powerful corporeal effects such as by disturbing visitors’ balance, stability, and upright position but instead creating experimental conditions by making participants aware of their body (e.g., via a body scan meditation, also see Dekeyzer, 2022 for a new meditation method based on proprioception to experience artworks starting from the body), or instructing one experimental group to specifically attend to their body while experiencing art. Future studies could also assess participants’ experience with body exercises (e.g., body scan meditation, yoga). Finally, we did not assess whether participants read the text about the installation artwork in the museum or on the museum website. Future studies should avoid this limitation.
We hope to have shown the value of adding qualitative assessments to the traditional quantitative assessment approach in empirical aesthetics, as the qualitative data revealed bodily experience dimensions that were not yet captured by our top-down selected body-items. Thus, this might help avoid biases on the researcher’s side. Our study is an example application of methodological triangulation (i.e., using different methodologies to approach the same topic; Todd et al., 2004). We recommend this mixed-methods design when investigating types of art experiences with installation art or other art genres that have yet to be systematically studied.
Our study also has broader implications. Unlocking the body’s role may be important for understanding the impact and value of art. Especially in times of increasing digital presence, installation art offers an in-person interaction and bodily experience, making it potentially a higher valued and comparably intense art experience. Indeed, contemporary art practice has increasingly turned toward the production of physical experiences (von Hantelmann, 2014; Kessels and Schrenk, 2022; Spence, 2022)—a movement that has been described as the “sensorial turn” (Levent et al., 2014, p. xvii), “experiential turn” (von Hantelmann, 2014), or “participation age” (Almenberg, 2010, p. 3). Insights on the role of heightened body awareness in experiencing profound or transformative states can help artists, curators, and museums to create more engaging and immersive experiences for visitors.
That said, with our theoretical review, we have made an argument for the central role of the body in the reception and appreciation of installation art. With our in situ ecologically valid case study, we showed that people report a wealth of bodily experiences when being asked about their experience, indicating a special role of proprioceptive and disturbing experiences for appreciation, with the latter specifically playing a role in transformative outcomes. Together, we hope to have planted the seed for a future embodied aesthetics of (installation) art that considers the multifaceted embodied responses we described.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Vienna Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
CK conceived the study, collected data, preprocessed and analyzed data, and wrote the manuscript. CK, MP, and JF designed the study. MP and JF edited and provided comments on the finalized manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
The writing of this study was supported by the Förderungsstipendium nach dem StudFG at the University of Vienna, funded by the Federal Ministry Republic of Austria for Education, Science and Research, and a grant to MP and JF from the EU Horizon 2020 TRANSFORMATIONS-17-2019, Societal Challenges and the Arts (870827—ARTIS, Art and Research on Transformations of Individuals of Society). JF’s work on this MS has been supported in part by Croatian Science Foundation under the project UIP-2020-021309.
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful to Falk Wolf (curator of K21) and the K21 (Kunstsammlung NRW, Düsseldorf, Germany) for access to the installation, Hannah Kruft for helping with the data collection, and Prof. Markus Schrenk, Juliane Zetschke, the PropArt team (Heinrich-Heine Universität, Düsseldorf), the EVA lab, and the ARTIS lab (University of Vienna), for valuable feedback on this study.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1192689/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Ackermann, M., and Saraceno, T., Hamburger Bahnhof – Museum für Gegenwart – Berlin Stiftung Kunstsammlung Nordrhein-Westfalen (eds.). (2011). Tomás Saraceno: Cloud Cities; [anläßlich der Ausstellung “Tomás Saraceno. Cloud Cities” vom 15. September 2011 bis 15. Januar 2012 in der Nationalgalerie im Hamburger Bahnhof, Museum für Gegenwart Berlin und der Installation “In den Umlaufbahnen” vom 10. Februar 2012 bis Herbst 2012 in der Kunstsammlung Nordrhein-Westfalen, K21 Ständehaus, Düsseldorf]. Berlin: Distanz-Verl.
Google Scholar
Almenberg, G. (2010). Notes on Participatory Art: Toward a Manifesto Differentiating It From Open Work, Interactive Art and Relational Art. Central Milton Keynes: AuthorHouse.
Google Scholar
Bengtsson, M. (2016). How to plan and perform a qualitative study using content analysis. NursingPlus Open 2, 8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.npls.2016.01.001
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bennett, N. P. (2019). Floating in space: disembodied experiences of being held tightly by the vast emptiness in Turrell’s “perfectly clear. PARtake: The Journal of Performance As Research. 2. doi: 10.33011/partake.v2i2.425
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and Psychobiology. New York, NY: Appleton Century Crofts.
Google Scholar
Best, S. (2002). What is affect? Considering the affective dimension of contemporary installation art. Aust. New Zealand J. Art 3, 207–225. doi: 10.1080/14434318.2002.11432712
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bingham, A. J., and Witkowsky, P. (2022). “Deductive and inductive approaches to qualitative data analysis,” in Analyzing and Interpreting Qualitative Data: After the Interview, eds C. Vanover, P. Mihas, and J. Saldaña (Los Angeles, LA: SAGE Publications), 133–146.
PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar
Bishop, C. (2005). Installation Art: A Critical History. London: Tate.
Google Scholar
Bornemann, B., Herbert, B. M., Mehling, W. E., and Singer, T. (2015). Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front. Psychol. 5, 1504. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01504
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bourriaud, N., Pleasance, S., and Woods, F. (2002). Relational Aesthetics. Dijon: les Presses du réel.
Google Scholar
Brieber, D., Nadal, M., and Leder, H. (2015). In the white cube: museum context enhances the valuation and memory of art. Acta Psychol. 154, 36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.004
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bukdahl, E. M. (2015). Embodied creation and perception in Olafur Eliasson’s and Carsten Höller’s projects. J. Somaesthet. 1, 160–181. doi: 10.5278/ojs.jos.v1i0.1076
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, A. P. (2018). NetworkToolbox: methods and measures for brain, cognitive, and psychometric network analysis in R. R J. 10, 422–439. doi: 10.32614/RJ-2018-065
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, A. P., Cardillo, E. R., and Chatterjee, A. (2022). What kind of impacts can artwork have on viewers? Establishing a taxonomy for aesthetic impacts. Br. J. Psychol. 114, 335–51. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/nt59q
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, A. P., Cotter, K. N., and Silvia, P. J. (2019). Reopening openness to experience: a network analysis of four openness to experience inventories. J. Pers. Assess. 101, 574–588. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2018.1467428
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, A. P., and Golino, H. (2021). Estimating the stability of psychological dimensions via Bootstrap Exploratory Graph Analysis: a Monte Carlo simulation and tutorial. Psych 3, 479–500. doi: 10.3390/psych3030032
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, A. P., Golino, H., and Silvia, P. J. (2020). A psychometric network perspective on the validity and validation of personality trait questionnaires. Eur. J. Pers. 34, 1095–1108. doi: 10.1002/per.2265
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Azevedo, R. T., and Tsakiris, M. (2021). Emotion matters: different psychophysiological responses to expressive and non-expressive full-body movements. Acta Psychol. 212, 103215. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103215
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Gaigg, S. B., and Calvo-Merino, B. (2018). I can feel my heartbeat: dancers have increased interoceptive accuracy. Psychophysiology 55, e13008. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13008
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Coburn, A., Vartanian, O., Kenett, Y. N., Nadal, M., Hartung, F., Hayn-Leichsenring, G., et al. (2020). Psychological and neural responses to architectural interiors. Cortex 126, 217–241. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.009
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cohen, A. B., Gruber, J., and Keltner, D. (2010). Comparing spiritual transformations and experiences of profound beauty. Psycholog. Relig. Spiritual. 2, 127–135. doi: 10.1037/a0019126
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cox, R. F. A., and van Klaveren, L.-M. (2022). The embodied experience of abstract art: an exploratory study. Open Sci. Frame. doi: 10.31219/osf.io/s4nry
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Crowther, P. (1993). Art and Embodiment: From Aesthetics to Self-consciousness. Oxford; New York, NY: Clarendon Press; Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Daubenmier, J. J. (2005). The relationship of yoga, body awareness, and body responsiveness to self-objectification and disordered eating. Psychol. Women Q. 29, 207–219. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00183.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dekeyzer, B. (2022). From within the body: proprioceptive art experiences in the museum. J. Vis. Liter. 41, 29–45. doi: 10.1080/1051144X.2021.1994731
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1934). Art as Experience. New York, NY: Perigee.
Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1980). Art as Experience. New York, NY: Perigee.
Google Scholar
Djebbara, Z., Fich, L. B., Petrini, L., and Gramann, K. (2019). Sensorimotor brain dynamics reflect architectural affordances. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 116, 14769–14778. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1900648116
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dufrenne, M. (1973). The Phenomenology of Aesthetic Experience. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.
Google Scholar
Eggart, M., Todd, J., and Valdés-Stauber, J. (2021). Validation of the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA-2) questionnaire in hospitalized patients with major depressive disorder. PLoS ONE 16, e0253913. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0253913
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O. J., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., and Borsboom, D. (2012). qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i04
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fingerhut, J., and Kühnapfel, C. (forthcoming). “Art embodied aesthetic emotions,” in “Being Moved to Interest”: Interestingness, Emotions 4E Cognition.
Google Scholar
Fingerhut, J., and Spee, B. T. M. (forthcoming). Aesthetic emotions, play, radical predictive processing of art. Philos. Transacti. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
Google Scholar
Finisguerra, A., Ticini, L. F., Kirsch, L. P., Cross, E. S., Kotz, S. A., and Urgesi, C. (2021). Dissociating embodiment and emotional reactivity in motor responses to artworks. Cognition 212, 104663. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104663
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2018). “Building a stronger concept of embodiment,” in The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition, eds A. Newen, L. De Bruin, and S. Gallagher (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 352–368.
PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar
Golino, H., Christensen, A., Moulder, R., Garrido, L. E., and Jamison, L. (2022). EGAnet: Exploratory Graph Analysis – a Framework for Estimating the Number of Dimensions in Multivariate Data using Network Psychometrics. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=EGAnet (accessed March 15, 2023).
PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar
Golino, H., Shi, D., Christensen, A. P., Garrido, L. E., Nieto, M. D., Sadana, R., et al. (2020). Investigating the performance of exploratory graph analysis and traditional techniques to identify the number of latent factors: a simulation and tutorial. Psychol. Methods 25, 292–320. doi: 10.1037/met0000255
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Golino, H. F., and Demetriou, A. (2017). Estimating the dimensionality of intelligence like data using Exploratory Graph Analysis. Intelligence 62, 54–70. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.02.007
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Golino, H. F., and Epskamp, S. (2017). Exploratory graph analysis: a new approach for estimating the number of dimensions in psychological research. PLoS ONE 12, e0174035. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0174035
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Greb, F., Elvers, P., and Fischinger, T. (2017). Trends in empirical aesthetics: a review of the journal Empirical Studies of the Arts from 1983 to 2014. Empir. Stud. Arts 35, 3–26. doi: 10.1177/0276237415625258
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gulhan, D., Durant, S., and Zanker, J. M. (2021). Similarity of gaze patterns across physical and virtual versions of an installation artwork. Sci. Rep. 11, 18913. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-91904-x
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hayn-Leichsenring, G. U., Kenett, Y. N., Schulz, K., and Chatterjee, A. (2020). Abstract art paintings, global image properties, and verbal descriptions: an empirical and computational investigation. Acta Psychol. 202, 102936. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.102936
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hobbs, R. (2001). “Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenology and installation art,” in Installations Mattress Factory 1990-1999, eds M. O. B. Luderowski, C. Giannini, and M. Factory (Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press).
Google Scholar
Jacucci, G., Spagnolli, A., Chalambalakis, A., Morrison, A., Liikkanen, L., Roveda, S., et al. (2009). “Bodily explorations in space: social experience of a multimodal art installation,” in Human-Computer Interaction – INTERACT 2009 Lecture Notes in Computer Science, eds. T. Gross, J. Gulliksen, P. Kotzé, L. Oestreicher, P. Palanque, R. O. Prates, et al. (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg),62–75.
Google Scholar
Jelić, A., and Staničić, A. (2021). “The memory in bodily and architectural making: reflections from embodied cognitive science,” in Affective Architectures: More-than-Representational Approaches to Heritage. Critical Studies in Heritage, Emotion and Affect. eds J. Micieli-Voutsinas and A. Person-Harm (London; New York: Routledge).
Google Scholar
Jelić, A., Tieri, G., De Matteis, F., Babiloni, F., and Vecchiato, G. (2016). the enactive approach to architectural experience: a neurophysiological perspective on embodiment, motivation, and affordances. Front. Psychol. 7, 481. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00481
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jones, A. (2000). “The 1970s “Situation” and recent installation: Joseph Santarromana’s intersubjective engagements,” in Space Site Intervention – Situating Installation Art, ed E. Suderburg (Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press).
Google Scholar
Jung, W.-M., Ryu, Y., Lee, Y.-S., Wallraven, C., and Chae, Y. (2017). Role of interoceptive accuracy in topographical changes in emotion-induced bodily sensations. PLoS ONE 12, e0183211. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0183211
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kai-Kee, E., Latina, L., and Sadoyan, L. (2020). Activity-Based Teaching in the Art Museum: Movement, Embodiment, Emotion. Los Angeles, CA: The J. Paul Getty Museum.
Google Scholar
Kapoula, Z., Lang, A., Lãa, T.-T., Adenis, M.-S., Yang, Q., Lipede, G., et al. (2014). Visiting Richard Serra’s “Promenade” sculpture improves postural control and judgment of subjective visual vertical. Front. Psychol. 5, 1349. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01349
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kühnapfel, C., and Fingerhut, J. (forthcoming). 4E Aesthetics: An Empirically Focused Review.
Google Scholar
Kühnapfel, C., Fingerhut, J., Brinkmann, H., Ganster, V., Tanaka, T., Specker, E., et al. (2023). How do we move in front of art? How does this relate to art experience? Linking movement, eye tracking, emotion, and evaluations in an ecologically-valid gallery setting. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/kx86d
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kyngäs, H. (2020). “Inductive content analysis,” in The Application of Content Analysis in Nursing Science Research, eds. H. Kyngäs, K. Mikkonen, and M. Kääriäinen (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 13–21.
Google Scholar
Levent, N. S., Pascual-Leone, A., and Lacey, S., (eds.). (2014). The Multisensory Museum: Cross-Disciplinary Perspectives on Touch, Sound, Smell, Memory, and Space. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.
Google Scholar
Levin, K., and Samaras, L. (1975). Lucas Samaras. New York, NY: H. N. Abrams.
Google Scholar
Massara, G. P., Di Matteo, T., and Aste, T. (2015). Network Filtering for Big Data: Triangulated Maximally Filtered Graph. doi: 10.48550/ARXIV.1505.02445
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
May, S., and Eliasson, O., (eds.). (2003). Olafur Eliasson: The weather project; [publ…. on the occasion of the exhibition at Tate Modern, London 16 October 2003 – 21 March 2004]. London: Tate Publ.
Google Scholar
Mehling, W. E., Acree, M., Stewart, A., Silas, J., and Jones, A. (2018). The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness, Version 2 (MAIA-2). PLoS ONE 13, e0208034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208034
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mehling, W. E., Gopisetty, V., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Hecht, F. M., and Stewart, A. (2009). Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PLoS ONE 4, 5614. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005614
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of Perception. London: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Miller, L. C., Murphy, R., and Buss, A. H. (1981). Consciousness of body: private and public. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 41, 397–406. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.2.397
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mills, C. M. (2009). “Materiality as the basis for the aesthetic experience in contemporary art,” in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers 1289 (Montana: University of Montana), p. 43.
Google Scholar
Mondloch, K. (2007). Be here (and there) now: the spatial dynamics of screen-reliant installation art. Art J. 66, 20–33. doi: 10.1080/00043249.2007.10791263
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Montero, B. (2006). Proprioception as an aesthetic sense. J. Aesthet. Art Crit. 64, 231–242. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-8529.2006.00244.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nather, F. C., Bueno, J. L. O., Abrew, D. C. C. C., and Gomes, M. M. (2010). “Body movements and timing estimation related to visual observation of different images representing distinct body positions,” in Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysics, 433–438.
Google Scholar
Newen, A., Bruin, L., and Gallagher, S., (eds.). (2018). The Oxford Handbook of 4E Cognition. 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Noë, A. (2000). Experience and experiment in art. J. Conscious. Stud. 7, 8–9.
Google Scholar
Noveck, J. (2017). At Yayoi Kusama Show, a Long Wait to Experience Infinity.
Google Scholar
Novitz, D. (2001). Participatory art and appreciative practice. J. Aesthet. Art Crit. 59, 153–165. doi: 10.1111/0021-8529.00015
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nusbaum, E. C., and Silvia, P. J. (2014). “Unusual aesthetic states,” in The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Aesthetics and the Arts, eds P. P. L. Tinio, and J. K. Smith (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 519–539.
Google Scholar
Oliveira, N., de Oxley, N., and Petry, M. (2003). Installation Art in the New Millennium: The Empire of the Senses. London: Thames and Hudson.
Google Scholar
Pelowski, M. (2011). Disruption, Change and Aesthetic Experience: A Theoretical Consideration, Cognitive Model and Empirical Study of Emotion, Appraisal, Understanding and Schema-Change in Transformative Encounters With Art. Nagoya: Nagoya University.
Google Scholar
Pelowski, M. (2012). Satori, koan and aesthetic experience: Exploring the “Realization of Emptiness” in Buddhist enlightenment via empirical study of modern art. Psyke and Logos. 33, 236–268.
Google Scholar
Pelowski, M. (2015). Tears and transformation: feeling like crying as an indicator of insightful or “aesthetic” experience with art. Front. Psychol. 6, 1006. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01006
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., and Akiba, F. (2011). A model of art perception, evaluation and emotion in transformative aesthetic experience. New Ideas Psychol. 29, 80–97. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2010.04.001
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., Hur, Y.-J., Cotter, K. N., Ishizu, T., Christensen, A. P., Leder, H., et al. (2021). Quantifying the if, the when, and the what of the sublime: a survey and latent class analysis of incidence, emotions, and distinct varieties of personal sublime experiences. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 15, 216–240. doi: 10.1037/aca0000273
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., Leder, H., Mitschke, V., Specker, E., Gerger, G., Tinio, P. P. L., et al. (2018). Capturing aesthetic experiences with installation art: an empirical assessment of emotion, evaluations, and mobile eye tracking in Olafur Eliasson’s “Baroque, Baroque!” Front. Psychol. 9, 1255. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01255
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., Liu, T., Palacios, V., and Akiba, F. (2014). When a body meets a body: an exploration of the negative impact of social interactions on museum experiences of art. Int. J. Educ. Arts 15, 1–48.
Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., Markey, P. S., Forster, M., Gerger, G., and Leder, H. (2017). Move me, astonish me… delight my eyes and brain: The Vienna Integrated Model of top-down and bottom-up processes in Art Perception (VIMAP) and corresponding affective, evaluative, and neurophysiological correlates. Phys. Life Rev. 21, 80–125. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2017.02.003
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., and Specker, E. (2020). “The general impact of context on aesthetic experience,” in The Oxford Handbook of Empirical Aesthetics, eds M. Nadal, and O. Vartanian (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 885–920.
Google Scholar
Pelowski, M., Specker, E., Gerger, G., Leder, H., and Weingarden, L. S. (2020). Do you feel like I do? A study of spontaneous and deliberate emotion sharing and understanding between artists and perceivers of installation art. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 14, 276–293. doi: 10.1037/aca0000201
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Petersen, A. R. (2015). “Between activism, installation art and relational aesthetics: Palle Nielsen’s The Model – Then and Now,” in The Model: Palle Nielsen, eds C. Gether, S. Høholt, D. J. Rugaard, T. Bechmann, N. M. Hansen (Arken: ARKEN, Museum for Moderne Kunst), 26–37.
Google Scholar
Porges, S. (1993). Body Perception Questionnaire. Maryland: Laboratory of Developmental Assessment.
Google Scholar
R Core Team (2022). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available online at: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed May 5, 2023).
Google Scholar
Rebentisch, J. (2012). Aesthetics of Installation Art. Berlin: Sternberg Pr.
Google Scholar
Reiss, J. H. (1999). From Margin to Center: The Spaces of Installation Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Robinson, J. (2012). On being moved by architecture. J. Aesthet. Art Crit. 70, 337–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6245.2012.01526.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Robinson, S. (2021). Architecture Is a Verb. New York, NY: Routledge.
Google Scholar
Rosenthal, S., (ed.). (2021). Yayoi Kusama: A Retrospective. New York, NY: Prestel.
Google Scholar
Ruiz, M. R. (2021). The contemplative walking in light somaesthetic experience in the projects of Ann Veronica Janssens and Olafur Eliasson. Debates Aesthet. 17, 51–65.
Google Scholar
Sadia, S. (2021). Empirical methodologies and the value of subjectivity in the analysis of the experience of contemporary experiential art. Art Percept. 9, 1–20. doi: 10.1163/22134913-bja10009
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sarasso, P., Neppi-Modona, M., Sacco, K., and Ronga, I. (2020). “Stopping for knowledge”: the sense of beauty in the perception-action cycle. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 118, 723–738. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.09.004
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Savaş, E. B., Verwijmeren, T., and van Lier, R. (2021). Aesthetic experience and creativity in interactive art. Art Percept. 9, 167–198. doi: 10.1163/22134913-bja10024
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schino, G., van Klaveren, L.-M., Gallegos González, H. G., and Cox, R. F. A. (2021). Applying bodily sensation maps to art-elicited emotions: an explorative study. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 1–15. doi: 10.1037/aca0000444
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schino, G., Van Klaveren, L.-M., Gallegos González, H. G., Cox, R. F. A., Nori, F., and Van Heusden, B. (2022). Measuring experiences of art in the museum: exploring methodology for getting it right. Itinera. doi: 10.54103/2039-9251/18562
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schneider, K. J. (2009). Awakening to Awe: Personal Stories of Profound Transformation. Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson.
Google Scholar
Schrenk, M. (2014). “Is proprioceptive art possible?,” in Philosophy and the Martial Arts, eds G. Priest, and D. Young (London; New Yor, NY: Routledge), 101–116.
Google Scholar
Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., and Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cogn. Emot. 21, 944–963. doi: 10.1080/02699930600923668
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Shusterman, R. (1999). Somaesthetics: a disciplinary proposal. J. Aesthet. Art Crit. 57, 299–313. doi: 10.1111/1540_6245.jaac57.3.0299
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., and Nusbaum, E. C. (2011). On personality and piloerection: Individual differences in aesthetic chills and other unusual aesthetic experiences. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 5, 208–214. doi: 10.1037/a0021914
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sone, Y. (2019). Fujiko Nakaya’s fog performance and embodied “nature”. Stud. Theatre Perform. 39, 165–176. doi: 10.1080/14682761.2018.1506965
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Specker, E., Forster, M., Brinkmann, H., Boddy, J., Pelowski, M., Rosenberg, R., et al. (2020). The Vienna Art Interest and Art Knowledge Questionnaire (Vaiak): A unified and validated measure of art interest and art knowledge. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts. 14, 172–185. doi: 10.1037/aca0000205
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Szubielska, M., and Imbir, K. (2021). The aesthetic experience of critical art: the effects of the context of an art gallery and the way of providing curatorial information. PLoS ONE 16, e0250924. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250924
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Szubielska, M., and Imbir, K. (2022). The aesthetic experience of contemporary installations in an art gallery and a laboratory setting: the issue of interactivity. ACP 18, 179–189. doi: 10.5709/acp-0360-7
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Szubielska, M., Imbir, K., and Szymańska, A. (2021). The influence of the physical context and knowledge of artworks on the aesthetic experience of interactive installations. Curr. Psychol. 40, 3702–3715. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00322-w
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thomas, D. R. (2006). A general inductive approach for analyzing qualitative evaluation data. Am. J. Eval. 27, 237–246. doi: 10.1177/1098214005283748
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tinio, P. P. L., and Gartus, A. (2018). Characterizing the emotional response to art beyond pleasure: correspondence between the emotional characteristics of artworks and viewers’ emotional responses. Prog. Brain Res. 237, 319–342. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.03.005
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Todd, Z., Nerlich, B., McKeown, S., and Clarke, D. D., (eds.). (2004). Mixing Methods in Psychology: The Integration of Qualitative and Quantitative Methods in Theory and Practice. London: Psychology Press.
Google Scholar
Tröndle, M., Greenwood, S., Kirchberg, V., and Tschacher, W. (2014). An integrative and comprehensive methodology for studying aesthetic experience in the field: merging movement tracking, physiology, and psychological data. Environ. Behav. 46, 102–135. doi: 10.1177/0013916512453839
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tröndle, M., and Tschacher, W. (2012). The physiology of phenomenology: the effects of artworks. Empir. Stud. Arts 30, 75–113. doi: 10.2190/EM.30.1.g
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Umiltà, M. A., Berchio, C., Sestito, M., Freedberg, D., and Gallese, V. (2012). Abstract art and cortical motor activation: an EEG study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 311. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00311
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Vartanian, O., and Skov, M. (2014). Neural correlates of viewing paintings: Evidence from a quantitative meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Brain Cogn. 87, 52–56. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2014.03.004
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Vernet, M., Morize, A., and Kapoula, Z. (2018). “Postural and emotional impact ofCarsten Höller’s artwork ‘Light Corner,”’ in Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences, eds Z. Kapoula, E. Volle, J. Renoult, and M. Andreatta (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 165–175.
Google Scholar
Vi, C. T., Ablart, D., Gatti, E., Velasco, C., and Obrist, M. (2017). Not just seeing, but also feeling art: Mid-air haptic experiences integrated in a multisensory art exhibition. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 108, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2017.06.004
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Vial Kayser, C., and Coëllier, S., (eds.). (2021). Installation Art as Experience of Self, in Space and Time. Wilmington, DE: Vernon Press.
Google Scholar
Vlemincx, E., Walentynowicz, M., Zamariola, G., Van Oudenhove, L., and Luminet, O. (2021). A novel self-report scale of interoception: the three-domain interoceptive sensations questionnaire (THISQ). Psychol. Health 1–20. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2021.2009479
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wattolik, E. (2019). Tomás Saraceno’s Art Work “In Orbit” (2013) against the backdrop of space architecture. Arts 8, 13. doi: 10.3390/arts8010013
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Weinberger, A. B., Christensen, A. P., Coburn, A., and Chatterjee, A. (2021). Psychological responses to buildings and natural landscapes. J. Environ. Psychol. 77, 101676. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101676
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Weingarden, L. S. (2014). “Installation art and experiential aesthetics: Mediating art history and neuroscience research,” in Proceedings of the Twenty-third Biennial Congress of the International Association of Empirical Aesthetics, eds V. A. Journeau, and C. V. Kayser (Vienna: Editions L’Harmattan).
Google Scholar
Weingarden, L. S. (2015). “Framing an experiential aesthetics – The affective turn in contemporary installation art,” in Notions esthétiques: La perception sensible organisée, eds V. A. Journeau, and C. Vial Kayser (Paris: Editions L’Harmattan).
Google Scholar
Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Vago, D. R., and Newberg, A. B. (2017). The varieties of self-transcendent experience. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 21, 143–160. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000102
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yoshitake, M., and Kusama, Y., Hirshhorn Museum Sculpture Garden (eds.). (2017). Yayoi Kusama: Infinity Mirrors. Washington, DC: Munich; New York: Hirshhorn Museum and Sculpture Garden; DelMonico Books/ Prestel.
Google Scholar